-
(单词翻译:双击或拖选)
SCIENCE IN THE NEWS -March 12, 2002: Babies and Intelligence
By George Grow
VOICE ONE:
This is Sarah Long.
VOICE TWO:
And this is Bob Doughty1 with SCIENCE IN THE NEWS, a VOA Special English program about recent
developments in science. Today, we discuss recent findings about how intelligence develops in babies.
((THEME)
)
VOICE ONE:
Not long ago, many people believed that babies only wanted food and to be kept warm and dry. Some people
thought babies were not able to learn things until they were five or six months old.
Yet doctors in the United States say babies begin learning on their first day of life.
The National Institute of Child Health and Development is an American government
agency. Its goal is to discover which experiences can influence healthy development
in humans.
Research scientists at the institute note that babies are strongly influenced by their
environment. They say a baby will smile if her mother does something the baby
likes. A baby learns to get the best care possible by smiling to please her mother or
other caregiver. This is how babies learn to connect and communicate with other humans.
VOICE TWO:
The American researchers say this ability to learn exists in a baby even before birth. They say newborn babies
can recognize and understand sounds they heard while they were still developing inside their mothers.
One study shows that babies can learn before they are born. The researchers placed a tape recorder on the
stomach of a pregnant2 woman. Then, they played a recording3 of a short story.
On the day the baby was born, the researchers tested to find out if he knew the sounds of the story repeated while
inside his mother. They did this by placing a device in the mouth of the newborn baby.
The baby would hear the story if he moved his mouth one way. If the baby moved his mouth the other way, he
would hear a different story. The researchers say the baby clearly liked the story he heard before he was born.
They say the baby would move his mouth so he could hear the story again and again.
VOICE ONE:
Researchers in Finland have shown that babies can learn while they are asleep. They demonstrated that newborn
babies can learn to identify different spoken sounds while sleeping.
The Finnish researchers divided forty-five newborns into three groups. They used devices to measures the
babies’
brain activity. The researchers played recordings4 of spoken sounds for up to one hour while the babies
slept
After this brief period, the researchers continued to play the recording to one group of babies during the night.
The second group heard a different recording. The third group did not hear any recording. The researchers
studied each baby ’s brain activity. Those in the first group could identify the sounds in the morning and again at
night. The other babies could not.
The head of the study believes that babies can learn while asleep because the part of their brains called the
cerebral5 cortex remains6 active at night. The cortex is very important for learning. This part of the brain is not
active in adults while they sleep.
((MUSIC BRIDGE))
VOICE TWO:
Many experts say the first years of a child ’s life are important for all later development. An American study
shows how mothers can strongly influence social development and language skills in their children.
The study involved more than one-thousand-two-hundred mothers and children. Researchers studied the children
from the age of one month to three years. They observed the mothers playing with their children four times
during this period.
VOICE ONE:
The researchers attempted to measure the sensitivity7 of the mothers. The women were considered sensitive if they
supported their children’s activities and did not interfere8 unnecessarily. They tested the children for thinking
and language development when they were three years old. Also, the researchers observed the women for signs
of the mental condition called depression.
The children of depressed9 women did not do as well on tests as the children of women who did not suffer from
depression. The children of depressed women did poorly on tests of language skills and understanding what they
hear.
These children also were less cooperative10 and had more problems dealing11 with other people. The researchers
noted12 that the sensitivity of the mothers was important to the general health of their children. Children did better
when their mothers were caring, even when the women suffered from depression.
((MUSIC BRIDGE))
VOICE TWO:
Another study suggests that babies who are bigger at birth generally are more intelligent later in life. It found that
the intelligence of a child at seven years of age is directly linked to his or her weight at birth. Study organizers
say this is probably because heavier babies received more nutrition during important periods of brain
development before they were born.
The study involved almost three-thousand-five -hundred children. Researchers in New York City used traditional
tests to measure intelligence. Brothers and sisters were tested so that the effects of birth weight alone could be
separated from the effects of diet or other considerations.
The researchers found that children with higher birth weights generally did better on the intelligence tests. Also,
the link between birth weight and intelligence later in life was stronger for boys than for girls.
VOICE ONE:
Another American study examined the development of very low birth weight babies. They were born early,
before the end of the normal nine -month development period.
Researchers in Cleveland, Ohio studied two-hundred-forty-two people who were born in the late Nineteen-
Seventies. At birth, they weighed an average of one-thousand-one-hundred-seventy-nine grams. On average, they
were born during the twenty-ninth week of pregnancy13. By comparison, a pregnancy is considered full term at
thirty-seven weeks.
VOICE TWO:
The researchers compared the progress of those born early with other children over a twenty-year period. They
found that the young people who had been very low birth weight babies were less likely to complete high school.
They also did not perform as well on intelligence tests as other adults.
However, the very low birth weight adults were less likely to use drugs or alcoholic14 drinks. They also were less
likely to become pregnant before the age of twenty.
((MUSIC BRIDGE))
VOICE ONE:
A long-term American study shows the importance of early education for poor children. The study is known as
the Abecedarian15 Project. It involved more than one-hundred young children from poor families in North
Carolina.
Half of the children attended an all-day program at a high-quality childcare center. The center offered
educational, health and social programs. Children took part in games and activities to increase their thinking and
language skills and social and emotional development.
The children attended the program from when they were a few weeks old until the age of five years. The other
group of children did not attend the childcare center. After the age of five, both groups attended public school.
VOICE TWO:
Researchers compared the two groups of children. When they were babies, both groups had similar results in tests
for mental and physical skills. However, from the age of eighteen months, the children in the educational child
care program did much better in tests.
The researchers tested the children again when they were twelve and fifteen years old. The tests found that the
children who had been in the childcare center continued to have higher average test results. These children did
much better on tests of reading and mathematics.
VOICE ONE:
Recently, organizers of the Abecedarian Project completed another examination of the students who are now
twenty-one years old. They were tested for thinking and educational ability, employment, parenting and social
skills.
The researchers found that the young adults who had the early education still did better in reading and
mathematics tests. They were more than two times as likely to be attending college or to have graduated from
college.
The study is more evidence that learning during the first months and years of life is important for all later
development.
((THEME))
VOICE TWO:
This VOA Special English program, SCIENCE IN THE NEWS, was written by George Grow. It was produced
by Cynthia Kirk. This is Bob Doughty.
VOICE ONE:
And this is Sarah Long. Join us again next week for more news about science in Special English on the Voice of
America.
Email this article to a friend
Printer Friendly Version
1 doughty | |
adj.勇猛的,坚强的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
2 pregnant | |
adj.怀孕的,怀胎的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
3 recording | |
n.录音,记录 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
4 recordings | |
n.记录( recording的名词复数 );录音;录像;唱片 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
5 cerebral | |
adj.脑的,大脑的;有智力的,理智型的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
6 remains | |
n.剩余物,残留物;遗体,遗迹 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
7 sensitivity | |
n.敏感(性),灵敏(度) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
8 interfere | |
v.(in)干涉,干预;(with)妨碍,打扰 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
9 depressed | |
adj.沮丧的,抑郁的,不景气的,萧条的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
10 cooperative | |
adj.有合作意向的,合作的;n.合作社(企业)等 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
11 dealing | |
n.经商方法,待人态度 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
12 noted | |
adj.著名的,知名的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
13 pregnancy | |
n.怀孕,怀孕期 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
14 alcoholic | |
adj.(含)酒精的,由酒精引起的;n.酗酒者 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
15 abecedarian | |
n.初学者;adj.字母的,初步的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|