-
(单词翻译:双击或拖选)
Though most of us spend a lifetime pursuing happiness, new research is showing that that goal may be largely out of our control. Two new studies this month add to a growing body of evidence that factors like genes1 and age may impact our general well-being2 more than our best day-to-day attempts at joy.
In one study, researchers at the University of Edinburgh suggest that genes account for about 50% of the variation in people's levels of happiness — the underlying3 determinant being genetically5 determined6 personality traits, like "being sociable7, active, stable, hardworking and conscientious," says co-author Timothy Bates. What's more, says Bates, these happiness traits generally come as a package, so that if you have one you're likely to have them all.
Bates and his Edinburgh colleagues drew their conclusions after looking at survey data of 973 pairs of adult twins. They found that, on average, a pair of identical twins shared more personality traits than a pair of non-identical twins. And when asked how happy they were, the identical twin pairs responded much more similarly than other twins, suggesting that both happiness and personality have a strong genetic4 component8. The study, published in Psychological Science, went one step further: it suggested that personality and happiness do not merely coexist, but that in fact innate9 personality traits cause happiness. Twins who had similar scores in key traits — extroversion10, calmness and conscientiousness11, for example — had similar happiness scores; once those traits were accounted for, however, the similarity in twins' happiness scores disappeared.
Another larger study, released in January ahead of its publication in Social Science & Medicine this month, shows that whatever people's individual happiness levels, we all tend to fall into a larger, cross-cultural and global pattern of joy. According to survey data representing 2 million people in more than 70 countries, happiness typically follows a U-shaped curve: among people in their mid-40s and younger, happiness trends downward with age, then climbs back up among older people. (That shift doesn't necessarily hold for the very old with severe health problems.) Across the world, people in their 40s generally claim to be less happy than those who are younger or older, and the global happiness nadir12 appears to hit somewhere around 44.
What happens at 44? Lots of things, but none that can be pinned down as the root cause of unhappiness. It's not anxiety from the kids, for starters. Even among the childless, those in midlife reported lower life satisfaction than the young or old, says study co-author Andrew Oswald, an economics professor at the University of Warwick in Britain. Other things that didn't alter the happiness curve: income, marital13 status or education. "You can adjust for 100 things and it doesn't go away," Oswald says. He and co-author David Blanchflower, an economist14 at Dartmouth College in New Hampshire, also adjusted their results for cohort effects: their data spanned more than 30 years, making them confident that whatever makes people miserable15 about being middle-aged16, it isn't related, say, to being born in the year 1960 and growing up with that generation's particular set of experiences.
At first glance, the new studies may appear at odds17 with some previous ones, largely because in happiness research, a lot depends on how you ask the question. Oswald and Blanchflower looked at responses to a sweeping18, general question: "Taken all together, how would you say things are these days — would you say that you are very happy, pretty happy or not too happy?" (The wording changes slightly depending on where the survey was conducted, but the question is essentially19 the same.) In a 2001 study, Susan Charles at University of California, Irvine, measured something slightly different: changes in positive affect, or positive emotions, versus20 negative affect over more than 25 years. Charles found that positive affect stayed roughly stable through young adulthood21 and midlife, falling off a little in older age; negative affect, meanwhile, fell consistently with age.
Charles thinks that feelings like angst, disgust and anger may fade because as we get older we learn to care less about what others think of us, or perhaps because we become more adept22 at avoiding situations we don't like. (The Edinburgh researchers, too, found that older study participants scored lower than younger ones on scales of neuroticism23 — worry and nervousness — and higher on scales of agreeableness.) Oswald chalks up the midlife dip in happiness shown in his study to people "letting go of impossible aspirations24" — first, there's the pain of fading youth and the realization25 that we may never accomplish all that we had dreamed, then the contentment we gain later in life through acceptance and self-awareness. "When you're young you can't do that," Oswald says.
An oft-cited finding from other happiness research suggests, however, that neither very good events nor very bad events seem to change people's happiness much in the long term. Most people, it seems, revert26 back to some kind of baseline happiness level within a couple years of even the most devastating27 events, like the death of a spouse28 or loss of limbs. Perhaps that kind of stability is due to heredity — those happiness-inducing personality traits that identical twins have been shown to share.
Still, lack of control doesn't necessarily mean lack of joy. "The research also shows that most people consider themselves happy most of the time," says University of Edinburgh's Bates. "We're wired to be optimistic. Most people think they're happier than most [other] people." And even if you aren't part of that lucky majority, Bates says, there's always that other 50% of overall life satisfaction that, according to his research, is not genetically predetermined. To feel happier, he recommends mimicking29 the personality traits of those who are: Be social, even if it's only with a few people; set achievable goals and work toward them; and concentrate on putting setbacks and worries in perspective. Don't worry, as the saying goes. Be happy.
点击收听单词发音
1 genes | |
n.基因( gene的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
2 well-being | |
n.安康,安乐,幸福 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
3 underlying | |
adj.在下面的,含蓄的,潜在的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
4 genetic | |
adj.遗传的,遗传学的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
5 genetically | |
adv.遗传上 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
6 determined | |
adj.坚定的;有决心的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
7 sociable | |
adj.好交际的,友好的,合群的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
8 component | |
n.组成部分,成分,元件;adj.组成的,合成的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
9 innate | |
adj.天生的,固有的,天赋的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
10 extroversion | |
n. [心理]外向,[医]外翻 =extraversion | |
参考例句: |
|
|
11 conscientiousness | |
责任心 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
12 nadir | |
n.最低点,无底 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
13 marital | |
adj.婚姻的,夫妻的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
14 economist | |
n.经济学家,经济专家,节俭的人 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
15 miserable | |
adj.悲惨的,痛苦的;可怜的,糟糕的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
16 middle-aged | |
adj.中年的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
17 odds | |
n.让步,机率,可能性,比率;胜败优劣之别 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
18 sweeping | |
adj.范围广大的,一扫无遗的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
19 essentially | |
adv.本质上,实质上,基本上 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
20 versus | |
prep.以…为对手,对;与…相比之下 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
21 adulthood | |
n.成年,成人期 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
22 adept | |
adj.老练的,精通的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
23 neuroticism | |
n.神经过敏症 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
24 aspirations | |
强烈的愿望( aspiration的名词复数 ); 志向; 发送气音; 发 h 音 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
25 realization | |
n.实现;认识到,深刻了解 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
26 revert | |
v.恢复,复归,回到 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
27 devastating | |
adj.毁灭性的,令人震惊的,强有力的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
28 spouse | |
n.配偶(指夫或妻) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
29 mimicking | |
v.(尤指为了逗乐而)模仿( mimic的现在分词 );酷似 | |
参考例句: |
|
|