-
(单词翻译:双击或拖选)
It has long been assumed that the cavalier behavior of teenagers — driving too fast, engaging in unprotected sex, dabbling1 in illicit2 drugs — is due in part to their characteristic disregard for mortality. Teens, as any beleaguered3 parent of one can attest4, usually operate under the presumption5 that they know it all and will live forever.
Or, do they? A new study published in the July issue of the journal Pediatrics would suggest precisely6 the opposite. For some teens, at least, their reckless, sometimes life-endangering choices are fueled not by feelings of being bulletproof, but by the belief that they're doomed7 to die young anyway.
In a long-term analysis of 20,594 American teens in grades 7 through 12, researchers interviewed the youngsters on three different occasions: first in 1995, again in 1996, then a final follow-up from 2000 to 2001. At the first interview, 1.4% of participants thought there was "almost no chance" that they'd reach their mid-30s; 2.4% thought it was possible, but hugely unlikely; and 10.9% believed they had only about a 50-50 shot of celebrating their 35th birthday. Researchers discovered that those who believed they were likely to die young were more likely to make potentially life-threatening choices — such as getting into violent fights or having unprotected sex with multiple partners — than teens who weren't expecting an early death.
"Thankfully most youths don't hold this belief," says lead author Dr. Iris8 Wagman Borowsky, an associate professor of pediatrics at the University of Minnesota, "but 15% did. That's one in seven youths in this country."Whether it was the risky9 behavior or the fatalistic worldview that presented itself first during the course of the study, Borowsky found they remained correlated over the years. Youths who reported that they had contemplated10 suicide, consistently gotten into fights, had unprotected sex or abused drugs by the time of the first interview in 1995 were more likely to develop a pessimistic attitude about their mortality during the subsequent interviews. Likewise, says Borowksy, "We found that those who felt they had a higher likelihood of dying early were more likely in later years to begin engaging in risky behaviors."What's more, having a negative view of the future varied11 widely among respondents, depending on their ethnicity, gender13 and socioeconomic status. Older male Hispanic adolescents were the most likely to believe their lives would be cut short. Among teens whose families received any form of financial assistance from the government, nearly one-quarter believed they were likely to die young.
The fact is that minorities and the underprivileged are among the populations in the U.S. who are statistically14 at higher risk of early death than, say, wealthy white Americans, according to government data. The irony15, Borowsky says, is that these fatalistic belief systems may help perpetuate16 the tendency toward poor health and early demise17 in certain social or ethnic12 groups. "What's disturbing to me is how this could contribute to health disparities among minorities as well as youths from different socioeconomic backgrounds," she says. "If youths are in an environment where they look around and see more adults dying early, then they may develop this perception that they will die early as well." And that may drive teens toward careless behaviors.
Borowsky's findings, while grim, present an opportunity to interrupt that self-fulfilling cycle (and she also found that as teens grow up, their negative views don't always persist). In the long term, she says, more research is needed for a deeper understanding of teens' emotional lives. But in the short term, prevention may be as simple as encouraging teenagers to think about their futures18 and set goals going forward; families and communities should then support children in achieving them.
"I think this is something that can take place in primary medical settings as well as school settings," Borowsky says. She believes we can make a difference — even save lives — just by asking teens one simple question: "What do you want to do when you get older?"
点击收听单词发音
1 dabbling | |
v.涉猎( dabble的现在分词 );涉足;浅尝;少量投资 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
2 illicit | |
adj.非法的,禁止的,不正当的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
3 beleaguered | |
adj.受到围困[围攻]的;包围的v.围攻( beleaguer的过去式和过去分词);困扰;骚扰 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
4 attest | |
vt.证明,证实;表明 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
5 presumption | |
n.推测,可能性,冒昧,放肆,[法律]推定 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
6 precisely | |
adv.恰好,正好,精确地,细致地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
7 doomed | |
命定的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
8 iris | |
n.虹膜,彩虹 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
9 risky | |
adj.有风险的,冒险的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
10 contemplated | |
adj. 预期的 动词contemplate的过去分词形式 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
11 varied | |
adj.多样的,多变化的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
12 ethnic | |
adj.人种的,种族的,异教徒的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
13 gender | |
n.(生理上的)性,(名词、代词等的)性 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
14 statistically | |
ad.根据统计数据来看,从统计学的观点来看 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
15 irony | |
n.反语,冷嘲;具有讽刺意味的事,嘲弄 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
16 perpetuate | |
v.使永存,使永记不忘 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
17 demise | |
n.死亡;v.让渡,遗赠,转让 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
18 futures | |
n.期货,期货交易 | |
参考例句: |
|
|